As our understanding of sentience in certain wildlife species grows, and technological advancements promote nonlethal data collection, we believe that we ought to adjust our field methods to incorporate a regime of best practice that prioritizes nonlethal methodologies over inhumane methods of lethal sampling. In addition, progress already made toward nonlethal methodologies in wildlife research needs to be promoted widely. In this paper, we examine whether lethal methods of whale research, using Japanese lethal scientific whaling as a case study, are ethical when the scientific information can be gained from nonlethal methods, and humane methods of killing are not available. As a part of a simple ethical decision-making model, we explore if a requirement for “refinement” of scientific technique, promoted extensively for laboratory-based animal experimentation, has direct applicability to scientific research involving free-living wildlife. We argue that refinement is an appropriate ethical principle in all cases where scientific research involves a choice between nonlethal sampling and the deliberate killing of free-living wildlife for scientific purposes. We conclude that the welfare of individual animals and the conservation of free-living wildlife populations are both worthy of moral consideration and need not be incompatible in humane wildlife research and management.

Use of animals in research remains controversial because of clear differences in how people perceive the relative worth of certain species in any calculation of known costs (in suffering) and future benefits (usually in terms of advances in human or animal health). When public attitudes toward the use of animals for experimental and teaching purposes are surveyed, the majority of people favor such practices so long as the procedures are important and suffering is minimized (e.g., Pifer et al. 1994; Franklin 2007; Crettaz von Roten 2012). Specific attitudes can differ from country to country, by gender, and how well informed survey respondents are (Swami et al. 2008; Ormandy and Schuppli 2014). The species of animal used can also influence public attitudes (Ormandy et al. 2012).

Most discussion of the moral status of research animals has centered on laboratory-based experimentation in biomedicine. It is only relatively recently that the increasing role of interventionist conservation research involving free-living wildlife has been the focus of ethical debate (e.g., Monamy 2007). Discussion of individual laboratory animal welfare and experimentation has expanded to include debate over the relative worth of free-living individuals and their interest in not suffering, together with the moral value of studying populations of wildlife species (e.g., Bekoff 2007).

Ethical discussions often refer to appropriate interactions between humans, but the terminology is no less valid when exploring interactions between humans and other animals. Ethos refers to a predominant community spirit from which a society's ethical bases are derived; morality refers to the distinction between right and wrong within that community spirit. In any ethical discussion, consequentialist objections to an action relate to the harmful or beneficial consequences of that action in a utilitarian context. An example; in March 2000, after public outcry, the Australian Government banned the use of hot-iron identification branding of seals and sea lions on the basis that an individual animal's interests (in avoiding acute pain) outweighed long-term conservation benefits in understanding certain population parameters of pinniped species once hunted to near extinction. The ban triggered numerous arguments for and against research involving iconic wildlife (Monamy 2007). Despite all ethical and legislative permissions being granted for the research, and all researchers working within the prescribed permissions' boundaries, all research was stopped when video footage of the branding was shown in the media (Monamy 2007). This illustrated that wildlife research, at least within an Australian ethos, was viewed by many in society as a privilege granted to scientists by the public via their bureaucracies, rather than a right. Of relevance to the present discussion, the ban on hot-iron branding may have hastened the development of effective noninvasive methodologies, such as photo identification based on whisker spot patterns, to gain the same data for ecological studies of free-living otariid species (Osterrieder et al. 2015).

The “three Rs” principles of humane experimental design (replacement, reduction, and refinement; Russell and Burch 1959) were developed to promote acceptable methodologies when using sentient animals in laboratory research. They have been adopted as the ethical foundation of modern animal experimentation in many parts of the world (Monamy 2009). Replacement proposes the use of nonsentient organisms rather than higher animals for experiments. Reduction refers to obtaining the best quality and most precise information with the smallest possible number of animals. Refinement, as an active principle, emphasizes keeping any pain, suffering, or other harm that may be caused to a research animal to a minimum. The principle emphasizes the need to balance costs and benefits of any proposed research to make sure that harm is absent or minimal and that benefits are maximal. The three Rs were originally written for laboratory animal experimentation; there were no formalized ethical parallels for research involving free-living wildlife. We argue, however, that refinement has particular relevance and applicability.

Debate about permissible scientific research involving wildlife generally focusses on the welfare of sentient beings and intensifies when invasive or environmentally disruptive research is conducted, or proposed, on an iconic, or highly valued, free-living species (Monamy 2007). Charismatic megafauna populations are especially valued by many nations for many reasons, including high aesthetic appeal, conservation status, certain economic values, or as symbols of cultural significance (Lavigne 2005). How, then, should we proceed when such iconic wildlife is subjected to invasive procedures, or when highly valued sentient species are killed for scientific data collection?

Case study: lethal scientific whaling in the Antarctic

Here we consider lethal scientific whaling in the Antarctic Ocean to illustrate the conflicting opinions of two nations' governments with strong views on the issue, Australia and Japan. Differing attitudes to animals among cultures have often led to intense debate about balancing intrinsic value and the instrumental worth of wildlife. Intrinsic value, in this sense, we define as the value an individual animal has “for its own sake” regardless of any value to humans. Here, instrumental worth refers to a value of wildlife to humans, as, for example, food, tourist opportunity, or research subject. Internationally, opinions on the relative value of whales have shifted increasingly toward prioritizing intrinsic value, with countries progressively abandoning whale hunting in favor of ecotourism opportunities that rely on ongoing conservation and management, particularly of vulnerable and threatened whale species (Bekoff 2007).

The management of whaling is governed by the International Convention for the Regulation of Whaling (ICRW), 1946, supported by the governments of 88 nations. Under this convention, a zero catch limit on commercial whaling (International Whaling Commission [IWC] 1946) came into effect in 1985, with three exceptions that allowed some whaling nations to continue harvesting. The exception provisions were utilized by: 1) Norway and Iceland, who continue to conduct commercial whaling under the provision that any member state could object to the ICRW within 90 d of its declaration and not be bound by it; 2) Alaska, Chukotka (Russian Far East), Greenland, and Bequia (West Indies), where Aboriginal subsistence whaling, which is categorized differently by the IWC and not subject to the moratorium, continues to take place; and 3) Japan, utilizing Article VIII of the ICRW to pursue special-permit whaling, or “scientific whaling” (see Table 1 for a summary of programs and objectives).

Table 1.

Objectives of past and future special-permit programs in the Antarctic Ocean by Japan (adapted from IWC 2015b).

Objectives of past and future special-permit programs in the Antarctic Ocean by Japan (adapted from IWC 2015b).
Objectives of past and future special-permit programs in the Antarctic Ocean by Japan (adapted from IWC 2015b).

The Japanese Whale Research Program under Special Permit in the Antarctic (JARPA) commenced in 1987, continuing until 2005. Japan then initiated the JARPA-II program (2005–2014). Between 1987 and 2014, Japan killed 10,710 whales under special-permit whaling in the Antarctic (IWC 2015a). The scientific value of the JARPA and JARPA-II programs came under intense scrutiny when Australia initiated proceedings against Japan in the International Court of Justice (ICJ) in the Hague in 2010 (ICJ 2010). The ICJ delivered the decision on the case “Whaling in the Antarctic (Australia v. Japan: New Zealand intervening)” on 31 March 2014, ruling that Japan's whaling was not for purposes of scientific research and that the Antarctic whaling program (JARPA-II) should cease immediately (ICJ 2014). The legal arguments by Japan and Australia have been discussed in detail elsewhere (e.g., De La Mare et al. 2014; Clapham 2015). Briefly, the question was whether JARPA-II was a commercial whaling operation that included some science (illegal within the ICRW framework), or was it conducted exclusively for scientific research (legal under ICRW)? Japan claimed that their whaling program was within the ICRW framework (i.e., exclusively for scientific research) and was, as such, legal. Australia argued that it was a method to circumvent the zero catch limit imposed on commercial whaling by ICRW and was not primarily for scientific research purposes (Clapham 2015). Despite the ICJ's landmark decision in favor of Australia's case against Japan (Simmonds 2014), it was predicted that the ICJ decision would only represent a temporary setback for Japanese whaling (Clapham 2015). Indeed, in November 2014 the government of Japan submitted a new special-permit proposal to the IWC scientific committee (New Scientific Research Program in the Antarctic Ocean). This research program then commenced in the 2015/2016 Antarctic season (IWC 2015b). Japan's rationale for lethal scientific whaling in the New Scientific Research Program in the Antarctic Ocean application stated that the importance of scientific research in the Antarctic Ocean is its “…potential of abundant living resources that could be sustainably exploited for food” (IWC 2015b). Japan continues to argue that nonlethal methodologies are not adequate to answer their research questions.

Modern nonlethal alternatives

Given that nonlethal methodologies in marine mammal research are highly advanced and readily utilized (see Hunt et al. 2013 for a comprehensive review), we argue that lethal methods are outdated and obsolete. Briefly, for example, in early dietary studies involving free-living marine wildlife, lethal sampling often was the simplest method available. However, recent studies involving stomach flushing of sharks (Barnett et al. 2010; Hammerschlag and Sulikowski 2011) and elephant seals (Daneri et al. 2015), fecal studies of fur seals (Gallo-Reynoso and Esperón-Rodríguez 2013) and penguins (Deagle et al. 2007), and stable isotope analysis of sea turtles (Shimada et al. 2014) all illustrate how nonlethal methodologies can be made best practice for marine species. In cetacean biology, whale abundance trends and biological parameters can be obtained via boat surveys, land-based observations, and mark–recapture studies. Recently, Peel et al. (2015) used passive acoustics to detect vocalizing whales (significantly increasing encounters when compared with visual detections alone), and, together with prioritizing biopsy data over photograph identification (when the choice arose), created a suitable method to estimate the abundance of circumpolar Antarctic blue whales (Balaenoptera musculus). Whale blow is increasingly being utilized for novel measures such as biogenic volatile organic compound profiling, which can be used to assess the health and physiological processes within an animal (Cumeras et al. 2014). Nonlethal whale blubber biopsies can be used for a large variety of studies and parameters including endocrinology (Vu et al. 2015), lipid and fatty acid and diet profiling (Waugh et al. 2012), as well as toxicological surveys (Waugh et al. 2014) and effects (Bengtson Nash et al. 2013). Whale skin samples can be used for studies such as enzyme activity and health biomarkers (Waugh et al. 2011; Bengtson Nash et al. 2014) and for epigenetic age estimation (Polanowski et al. 2014). These are just a few examples of the novel technologies that are continuing to expand the wide field of nonlethal marine mammal research.

Yet, the question then is, will mandating protection from harm for individuals, by limiting the availability of lethal methods, necessarily mean that long-term conservation and management of vulnerable populations are jeopardized? We do not believe so. Already the momentum shift to nonlethal methodologies, which is already occurring, continues to stimulate the advancement of novel and ethical techniques. The banning of invasive identification techniques in some species of wildlife has already stimulated a search for an alternative. This can result in more accurate methods, such as face recognition software for gorilla conservation research (Loos et al. 2011). Specifically for whales, a key example is the historical difficulty in accurately ageing large free-swimming cetacean species; this is one of Japan's main arguments for their continuation of lethal sampling (IWC 2015b). A search for a nonlethal alternative led to the development of a novel technique utilizing whale skin to form an epigenetic age estimation method (Polanowski et al. 2014). This technology was based on assays for human age that had recently been developed using age-induced changes in DNA methylation of specific genes (e.g., Hannum et al. 2013). The subsequent humpback epigenetic age assay developed by Polanowski et al. (2014) predicted age (±3 yr) from skin samples (using the three most age-informative cytosine markers). Despite this, Japan continues to argue that “DNA-based methods are not practicable” (IWC 2015b), and that counting of growth-layer groups accumulated in the earplug of whales (only accessible via lethal methods) is the only reliable method. However, there is measurement error in this method (Chittleborough 1959), with a lack of consensus on how many growth-layer groups are produced each year (Chittleborough 1959; Lockyer 1984; Gabriele et al. 2010). The most reliable method for age estimation remains visual identification of individuals that were first seen as calves (Polanowski et al. 2014). Using this example, we urge that any argued benefit of counting growth-layer groups over humpback epigenetic age assays does not outweigh the individual suffering caused by harpooning, which by general and scientific standards is deemed inhumane.

Individual welfare and lethal scientific whaling

Lethal sampling of free-swimming whales after harpooning comes at a high cost to individual animals in terms of suffering before death. With data on Japanese killing methods showing that time to death averaged 10 min, with some animals taking up to 25 min to die (Gales et al. 2008), we believe that the continued use of these methods is morally indefensible. Instantaneous death rates are extremely low and time to death are too high (Gales et al. 2008). Comparisons with best practice in the lethal scientific sampling of free-living terrestrial wildlife, in recreational hunting, and in the humane slaughter of food animals show unacceptably low instantaneous death rates and unacceptably high time to death in lethal scientific whaling.

The three Rs principles of humane experimental design (Russell and Burch 1959) provide a framework to minimize suffering, and have not only been adopted as the foundation of modern laboratory animal experimentation among Western nations (Monamy 2009), but are also part of the Japan's regulation of animal research (Japanese Association for Laboratory Animal Science, JALAS 2013). Refinement as a governing ethical principle emphasizes keeping any pain, suffering, or other harm that may be caused to a minimum for each animal used in research. We believe Japan's continued use of lethal whaling methods needs to cease, given that there are proven nonlethal methods available (e.g., epigenetic age estimation). The extent of pain, suffering, and distress to the individual (inhumane killing methods) and lasting harm that might be caused to Antarctic whale stocks (which are still at a small fraction of their prewhaling levels) must thus be assessed, and methods put in place to determine the best ways to minimize these effects (i.e., a ban on lethal scientific whaling).

Nonlethal sampling and conservation biology

Refinement emphasizes the need to balance costs and benefits of any proposed research to make sure that any harm to an animal is minimized. So, does that mean that all invasive research involving free-living wildlife should cease? We do not believe so. In the case of scientific whaling, if the welfare costs are weighed and compared with the benefits that long-term population studies provide to the conservation of a species, then nonlethal sampling is demonstrably the best practice and such studies ought to replace all lethal studies. We argue, then, for the continued advancement of wildlife research based on a simple ethical model that privileges individual welfare over conservation outcomes by replacing inhumane lethal methodologies with nonlethal sampling. In a situation where nonlethal methods are not available and humane killing is not achievable, then such research should not be permitted until nonlethal methods have been developed. Using such an ethical model, Japan's lethal sampling of whale populations in the Southern Ocean is unethical because nonlethal methodologies exist (e.g., Polanowski et al. 2014) and because the instantaneous death rates and time to death from harpooning are inhumane. The use of lethal methods by Japanese researchers in the Southern Ocean does not adhere to the refinement principle (Russell and Burch 1959), as adopted by JALAS (2013), and the majority of the Western scientific community (e.g., National Research Council [US] Institute for Laboratory Animal Research 2004).

Implications for conservation and management

We believe that current research approaches ought to continue the shift toward prioritizing nonlethal data collection methods in wildlife research. We urge that the ICRW (IWC 1946) be newly negotiated by members of the IWC to exclude Article VIII so that lethal scientific whaling is banned. With further advances in nonlethal wildlife research being reported, we support a shift of thinking toward a new ethically attuned paradigm of modern wildlife research. This has applicability for terrestrial wildlife research as well. For example, in dietary studies, nonlethal sampling of salamanders (Crovetto et al. 2012; Costa et al. 2015) and lizards (Crovetto and Salvidio 2013) by stomach flushing and fecal analyses has already replaced the lethal methods once used. Lethal methods may be traditional in some cultures and conservation disciplines but that doesn't make them a moral “right” within a global community, or a “necessary evil” through which information is gathered. Research must continue to move our understanding of wild animals and their effective conservation and management forward, but only in pace with the advancement of nonlethal methodologies. It must continually strive toward improved data collection methods incorporating a regime of best practice that acknowledges ethical as well as technical advancements. Where a nonlethal methodology is poorly developed and lethal techniques are not humane, research questions ought to go unanswered until an acceptable nonlethal method is developed. Adherence to the ethical principle of refinement ensures that nonlethal research techniques, once available, take precedence over lethal data collection in conservation biology.

We acknowledge the journal Associate Editor and two anonymous reviewers whose comments significantly improved the manuscript.

Any use of trade, product, or firm names is for descriptive purposes only and does not imply endorsement by the U.S. Government.

Barnett
A,
Redd
KS,
Frusher
SD,
Stevens
JD,
Semmens
JM.
2010
.
Non-lethal method to obtain stomach samples from a large marine predator and the use of DNA analysis to improve dietary information
.
Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology
393
:
188
192
.
Bekoff
M.
2007
.
Aquatic animals, cognitive ethology, and ethics: questions about sentience and other troubling issues that lurk in turbid water
.
Diseases of Aquatic Organisms
75
:
87
98
.
Bengtson Nash S, Dawson A, Burkhard M, Waugh C, Huston W
.
2014
.
Detoxification enzyme activities (CYP1A1 and GST) in the skin of humpback whales as a function of organochlorine burdens and migration status
.
Aquatic Toxicology
155
:
207
212
.
Bengtson Nash S, Waugh CA, Schlabach M
.
2013
.
Metabolic concentration of lipid soluble organochlorine burdens in the blubber of southern hemisphere humpback whales through migration and fasting
.
Environmental Science and Technology
47
:
9404
9413
.
Chittleborough
RG.
1959
.
Determination of age in the humpback whale, Megaptera nodosa (Bonnaterre)
.
Marine and Freshwater Research
10
:
125
143
.
Clapham
PJ.
2015
.
Japan's whaling following the International Court of Justice ruling: brave new world—or business as usual?
Marine Policy
51
:
238
241
.
Costa
A,
Salvidio
S,
Posillico
M,
Matteucci
G,
De Cinti
B,
Romano
A.
2015
.
Generalisation within specialization: inter-individual diet variation in the only specialized salamander in the world. Scientific Reports 5, article number: 13260
(2015) doi:.
Crettaz von Roten F
.
2012
.
Public perceptions of animal experimentation across Europe
.
Public Understanding of Science
22
:
691
703
.
Crovetto
F,
Romano
A,
Salvidio
S.
2012
.
Comparison of two nonlethal methods for dietary studies in terrestrial salamanders
.
Wildlife Research
39
:
266
270
.
Crovetto
F,
Salvidio
S.
2013
.
Feeding habits of the sand lizard, Lacerta agilis, from North-Western Italian Alps
.
Folia Zoologica
62
:
264
268
.
Cumeras
R,
Cheung
WHK,
Gulland
F,
Goley
D,
Davis
CE.
2014
.
Chemical analysis of whale breath volatiles: a case study for non-invasive field health diagnostics of marine mammals
.
Metabolites
4
:
790
806
.
Daneri
GA,
Carlini
AR,
Marschoff
ER,
Harrington
A,
Negrete
J,
Mennucci
JA,
Márquez
MEI.
2015
.
The feeding habits of the Southern elephant seal, Mirounga leonina, at Isla 25 de Mayo/King George Island, South Shetland Islands
.
Polar Biology
38
:
665
676
.
Deagle
BE,
Gales
NJ,
Evans
K,
Jarman
SN,
Robinson
S,
Trebilco
R,
Hindell
MA.
2007
.
Studying seabird diet through genetic analysis of faeces: a case study on macaroni penguins (Eudyptes chrysolophus)
.
PLoS ONE
2
(
9
):
e831
. Doi: .
De La Mare
W,
Gales
N,
Mangel
M.
2014
.
Applying scientific principles in international law on whaling
.
Science
345
(
6201
):
1125
1126
.
Franklin
A.
2007
.
Human–nonhuman animal relationships in Australia: an overview of results from the first national survey and follow-up case studies 2000–2004
.
Society and Animals
15
:
7
27
.
Gabriele
CM,
Lockyer
C,
Straley
JM,
Jurasz
CM,
Kato
H.
2010
.
Sighting history of a naturally marked humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) suggests ear plug growth layer groups are deposited annually
.
Marine Mammal Science
26
:
443
450
.
Gales
N,
Leaper
R,
Papastavrou
V.
2008
.
Is Japan's whaling humane?
Marine Policy
32
:
408
412
.
Gallo-Reynoso
JP,
Esperón-Rodríguez
M.
2013
.
Diet composition of the Guadalupe fur seal (Arctocephalus townsendi). Where and what do they eat?
Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology
46
:
455
467
.
Hammerschlag
N,
Sulikowski
J.
2011
.
Killing for conservation: the need for alternatives to lethal sampling of apex predatory sharks
.
Endangered Species Research
14
:
135
140
.
Hannum
G,
Guinney
J,
Zhao
L,
Zhang
L,
Hughes
G,
Sadda
S,
Klotzle
B,
Bibikova
M,
Fan
JB,
Gao
Y,
Deconde
R,
Chen
M,
Rajapakse
I,
Friend
S,
Ideker
T,
Zhang
K.
2013
.
Genome-wide methylation profiles reveal quantitative views of human aging rates
.
Molecular Cell
49
:
359
367
.
Hunt
KE,
Moore
MJ,
Rolland
RM,
Kellar
NM,
Hall
AJ,
Kershaw
J,
Raverty
SA,
Davis
CE,
Yeates
LC,
Fauquier
DA,
Rowles
TK,
Kraus
SD.
2013
.
Overcoming the challenges of studying conservation physiology in large whales: a review of available methods
.
Conservation Physiology
1
:
1
24
. doi: .
[ICJ] International Court of Justice
.
2010
.
Australia institutes proceedings against Japan for alleged breach of international obligations concerning whaling. International Court of Justice (ICJ), The Hague, Netherlands
.
Available
: http:// http://www.icj-cij.org/docket/files/148/15953.pdf (January 2016).
ICJ
.
2014
.
Whaling in the Antarctic (Australia v. Japan: New Zealand intervening)—judgment of 31 March 2014
.
International Court of Justice (ICJ)
,
The Hague, Netherlands
. .
[IWC] International Whaling Commission
.
1946
.
International Convention for the Regulation of Whaling, 1946 Schedule
.
Reports of the International Whaling Commission (IWC), 38: Para 10e. Available: https://archive.iwc.int/pages/view.php?ref=3607&k= (January 2016)
.
IWC
.
2015
a
.
Catches taken: Special Permit. Special permit catches since 1985
.
In International Whaling Commission (IWC). Available: https://iwc.int/table_permit (January 2016)
.
IWC
.
2015
b
.
Special permit programmes
.
In International Whaling Commission (IWC). Available: https://iwc.int/spw-programmes (January 2016)
.
[JALAS] Japanese Association for Laboratory Animal Science
2013
.
Core values statement
. .
Lavigne
DM.
2005
.
Marine mammals and fisheries: the role of science in the culling debate
.
Pages
31
47
in
Gales
N,
Hindell
M
Kirkwood
R,
editors
.
Marine mammals: fisheries, tourism and management issues
.
Victoria, Australia
:
CSIRO Publishing
,.
Lockyer
C.
1984
.
Age determination by means of the earplug in baleen whales
.
Report of the International Whaling Commission
34
:
692
696
.
Loos
A,
Pfitzer
M,
Aporius
L.
2011
.
Identification of Great Apes using face recogniton. 19th European Signal Processing Conference (EUSIPCO 2011)
.
Barcelona
,
Spain
,
pp
.
922
926
.
Monamy
V.
2007
.
Hot iron branding of seals and sea lions: why the ban will remain
.
Australian Veterinary Journal
85
:
485
486
.
Monamy
V.
2009
.
Animal experimentation: a guide to the issues. 2nd edition
.
Cambridge, UK
:
Cambridge University Press
.
National Research Council (US) Institute for Laboratory Animal Research
.
2004
.
The Development of Science-Based Guidelines for Laboratory Animal Care: Proceedings of the November 2003 International Workshop
.
Washington, DC
:
National Academies Press (US)
. .
Ormandy
EH,
Schuppli
CA.
2014
.
Public attitudes toward animal research: a review
.
Animals
4
:
391
408
.
Ormandy
EH,
Schuppli
CA,
Weary
DM.
2012
.
Factors affecting people's acceptance of the use of zebrafish and mice in research
.
Alternatives to Laboratory Animals
40
:
321
333
.
Osterrieder
SK,
Kent
CS,
Anderson
CJR,
Parnum
IM,
Robinson
RW.
2015
.
Whisker spot patterns: a noninvasive method of individual identification of Australian sea lions (Neophoca cinerea)
.
Journal of Mammalogy
96
:
988
997
.
Peel
D,
Bravington
M,
Kelly
N,
Double
MC.
2015
.
Designing an effective mark–recapture study of Antarctic blue whales
.
Ecological Applications
25
:
1003
1015
.
Pifer
L,
Shimizu
K,
Pifer
R.
1994
.
Public attitudes toward animal research: some international comparisons
.
Society and Animals
2
:
95
113
.
Polanowski
AM,
Robbins
J,
Chandler
D,
Jarman
SN.
2014
.
Epigenetic estimation of age in humpback whales
.
Molecular Ecology Resources
14
:
976
987
.
Russell
WMS,
Burch
RL.
1959
.
The principles of humane experimental technique
.
London
:
Methuen
.
Shimada
T,
Aoki
S,
Kameda
K,
Hazel
J,
Reich
K,
Kamezaki
N.
2014
.
Site fidelity, ontogenetic shift and diet composition of green turtles Chelonia mydas in Japan inferred from stable isotope analysis
.
Endangered Species Research
25
:
151
164
.
Simmonds
MP.
2014
.
Landmark ruling on whaling from the International Court of Justice
.
Antarctic Science
26
:
329
.
Swami
V,
Furnham
A,
Christopher
AN.
2008
.
Free the animals? Investigating attitudes toward animal testing in Britain and the United States
.
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology
49
:
269
276
.
Vu
ET,
Casey
C,
Catelani
K,
Kellar
NM,
Calambokidis
J.
2015
.
Seasonal blubber testosterone concentrations of male humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae)
.
Marine Mammal Science
31
:
1258
1264
.
Waugh
CA,
Huston
WM,
Noad
MJ,
Bengtson Nash
S.
2011
.
Cytochrome P450 isozyme protein verified in the skin of southern hemisphere humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae): implications for biochemical biomarker assessment
.
Marine Pollution Bulletin
62
:
758
761
.
Waugh
CA,
Nichols
PD,
Noad
MC,
Bengtson Nash
S.
2012
.
Lipid and fatty acid profiles of migrating Southern Hemisphere humpback whales Megaptera novaeangliae
.
Marine Ecology Progress Series
471
:
271
281
.
Waugh
CA,
Nichols
PD,
Schlabach
M,
Noad
M,
Bengtson Nash
S.
2014
.
Vertical distribution of lipids, fatty acids and organochlorine contaminants in the blubber of southern hemisphere humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae)
.
Marine Environmental Research
94
:
24
31
.

Author notes

Citation: Waugh CA, Monamy V. 2016. Opposing lethal wildlife research when nonlethal methods exist: scientific whaling as a case study. Journal of Fish and Wildlife Management 7(1):231-236; e1944-687X. doi: 10.3996/072015-JFWM-061

The findings and conclusions in this article are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily represent the views of the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service.