Background

Racism is a longstanding driver of health inequities. Although medical education is a potential solution to address racism in health care, best practices remain unknown.

Objective

We sought to evaluate the impact of participation in a curriculum addressing racism on pediatric residents' racial biases and empathy.

Methods

A pre-post survey study was conducted in 2 urban, university-based, midsized pediatric residency programs between July 2019 and June 2020. The curriculum sessions included Self-Reflection on Implicit Bias, Historical Trauma, and Structural Racism. All sessions were paired with empathy and perspective-taking exercises and were conducted in small groups to facilitate reflective discussion. Wilcoxon signed rank tests were used to assess changes in racial bias and empathy. Linear regression was used to assess the effect of resident characteristics on racial bias and empathy.

Results

Ninety of 111 residents receiving the curriculum completed pre-surveys (81.1%), and among those, 65 completed post-surveys (72.2%). Among participants with baseline pro-White bias, there was a statistically significant shift (0.46 to 0.36, P=.02) toward no preference. Among participants with a baseline pro-Black bias, there was a statistically significant shift (-0.38 to -0.21, P=.02), toward no preference. Among participants with baseline pro-White explicit bias, there was a statistically significant shift (0.54 to 0.30, P<.001) toward no preference. Among all residents, there was a modest but statistically significant decrease in mean empathy (22.95 to 22.42, P=.03).

Conclusions

Participation in a longitudinal discussion-based curriculum addressing racism modestly reduced pediatric residents' racial preferences with minimal effects on empathy scales.

Interpersonal racism, including physicians' racial biases, contributes to disparate care.1  The Association of American Medical Colleges (AAMC) has therefore called for the development of education promoting racial equity,2  and the Accreditation Council for Graduate Medical Education (ACGME) requires residents to demonstrate sensitivity to race.3 

Educational approaches to address racial inequities have varied, with focuses on cultural competency,4  cultural humility,5  advocacy,6  or health disparities.7  While there has been a shift toward addressing racial biases,8  few existing interventions directly address racism.9  Such omission fails to recognize racism's societal impact and can reinforce biases that perpetuate racial inequities.10  Additionally, recent findings highlight the role of empathy in mitigating the consequences of racial bias within health care.11  While empathy is often taught in medical education, the role of empathy within the context of racism curricula has not been investigated. Curricular evaluations have also focused predominantly on knowledge and satisfaction, rather than attitudes and skills, and often use evaluation tools with limited evidence.12 

We aimed to address these gaps by evaluating the impact of a curriculum, explicitly focused on racism, on residents' racial biases and empathy.

Setting

This pre-post survey study was conducted between July 2019 and June 2020 at 2 urban, university-based, midsized pediatric residency programs. The curriculum was a mandatory component of existing rotations, but research participation was voluntary. Residents received the pre-curriculum survey 1 week before their rotations. Residents who completed the pre-survey were sent the post-survey after completing their rotations.

Curriculum Development

Using Kern's model,13  the first author (M.J.) created the curriculum, which consists of 3 one-hour lectures, including Self-Reflection on Implicit Bias, Historical Trauma, and Structural Racism (provided as online supplementary data). Each lecture was supplemented with empathy and perspective-taking exercises. Facilitator guides were used to minimize site-to-site variation.

Curriculum Evaluation

The independent variable was curriculum exposure. The dependent variables were racial biases (implicit and explicit) and empathy (empathic concern and perspective-taking). Implicit bias was measured using the race Implicit Association Test (IAT), which assesses unconscious attitudes by measuring the speed with which an individual pairs positive or negative concepts with Black or White faces.14  Explicit bias was assessed using a composite score of the race preference scale and feeling thermometer scale.15,16  Empathy was measured by the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI), a tool with validity evidence18  that is used within medical settings17  and education research.19  Two of 4 subscales, empathic concern and perspective-taking, were selected given their relationship to bias mitigation.11,20  Order of bias and empathy measures were randomized for each participant, and IAT results were revealed after survey completion.

We also collected sociodemographic data (age, sex, race/ethnicity, childhood household income, and geography based on US Census regions)21  and training characteristics (institution, training level, residency program, intended career path, previous bias training, and burnout).22 

Data Analysis

We performed descriptive analyses with analyses of variance to associate key covariates with dependent variables. We compared pre-post responses using Wilcoxon signed rank tests to assess the impact of the curriculum on racial bias and empathy. We then assessed associations between each dependent variable and key covariates selected based on prior research, followed by multivariable linear regression.

The institutional review boards at both sites approved this study.

Of 111 residents who received the curriculum, 90 completed pre-surveys (81.1%) and 65 completed post-surveys (72.2%). The Table summarizes sample characteristics. There were no significant differences between those who completed both surveys and those who only completed the pre-survey.

Table

Characteristics of Residents Exposed to the Racism Curriculum

Characteristics of Residents Exposed to the Racism Curriculum
Characteristics of Residents Exposed to the Racism Curriculum

Figures 1a and 1b display changes in bias following curriculum exposure. Participants with baseline pro-White implicit bias shifted (0.46 to 0.36, P=.02) toward no preference. Participants with a baseline pro-Black implicit bias shifted (-0.38 to -0.21, P=.02) toward no preference. Participants with baseline pro-White explicit bias shifted (0.54 to 0.30, P<.001) toward no preference. Among all residents, mean empathy decreased (22.95 to 22.42, P=.03).

Figure 1a

Change in Implicit Bias Following Curriculum

Note: Possible implicit bias scores range from –2 to +2. Scores <-0.15 indicate pro-Black bias. Scores -0.15-0.15 indicate no bias with 0 being entirely no preference. Scores >0.15 indicate pro-White bias.

a Denotes P<.05.

Figure 1a

Change in Implicit Bias Following Curriculum

Note: Possible implicit bias scores range from –2 to +2. Scores <-0.15 indicate pro-Black bias. Scores -0.15-0.15 indicate no bias with 0 being entirely no preference. Scores >0.15 indicate pro-White bias.

a Denotes P<.05.

Close modal
Figure 1b

Change in Explicit Bias Following Curriculum

Note: Possible explicit bias scores range from -2 to +2 with negative scores indicating pro-Black bias, 0 indicating no preference, and positive scores indicating pro-White bias.

a Denotes P<.01.

Figure 1b

Change in Explicit Bias Following Curriculum

Note: Possible explicit bias scores range from -2 to +2 with negative scores indicating pro-Black bias, 0 indicating no preference, and positive scores indicating pro-White bias.

a Denotes P<.01.

Close modal

Online supplementary data Table 1 shows associations between resident characteristics and dependent variables before curriculum exposure. Pre-curriculum implicit bias was associated with childhood household income. Pre-curriculum empathic concern was associated with sex. Pre-curriculum perspective-taking was associated with childhood household income and geography.

Online supplementary data Table 2 displays associations between resident characteristics and changes in bias and empathy. Significant associations were demonstrated with younger age, White race, growing up in the Northeast, and growing up in a household with an income >$150,000 per year.

The shift from pro-White or pro-Black implicit bias toward no preference in our study adds to evidence demonstrating the effectiveness of curricular interventions on bias mitigation.23,24  Only one other study within medical literature, to our knowledge, has used an objective measure (eg, IAT), rather than self-report measures,25  but found no changes in bias.22 

The shift demonstrated in explicit bias following the curriculum is unique and has not been previously reported.23  This finding may be attributed to a change in the national climate amid COVID-19 racial disparities and widely publicized police violence against Black Americans, resulting in increased activity within movements such as Black Lives Matter, which has been shown to shift explicit biases.26  However, these shifts in bias may also relate to the curriculum's emphasis on historical context and structural forces that sustain racism, thereby transferring attention from bias against patients to interrogation of the system.

Next, although implicit and explicit bias decreased when examining the full sample, these findings were not statistically significant. This may be due to small sample size.24  Additionally, our sample may have included “deniers” (ie, individuals who do not agree that biases impact care),27  who are more challenging to teach about racial biases.28 

While the findings regarding shifting biases are promising, the decrease in empathic concern was unexpected. While empathy can facilitate positive inter-group relations, empathy promotion among those with bias may activate stereotypes rather than minimize them.29  Additionally, empathy may worsen with burnout.30  Although burnout may have been a relevant confounder, we did not collect post-survey data on burnout, precluding our ability to assess this relationship. These results should, however, also be interpreted with caution given the small magnitude of change between the values—as statistical significance does not always translate to clinical significance.

Lastly, our findings show specific resident characteristics were significantly associated with changes in bias and empathy. Younger residents showed decreases in implicit bias and increases in empathy. Older age is not only associated with implicit bias, which may be due to diminished ability to suppress automatic associations,31  but also lower levels of empathy.32  This suggests the importance of education early in training. Growing up in a low-income household was associated with decreases in implicit bias. This contrasts with research demonstrating a relationship between individuals with lower income and higher levels of explicit racial bias, but not with implicit bias.33  Residents from low-income households may have formed a sense of shared identity through feeling “othered” by the dominant group, a bias mitigating strategy.28  White race was associated with increases in explicit bias, consistent with prior literature.34  Perhaps the curriculum increased White residents' awareness of their biases and reporting of their explicit biases in post-surveys, a mechanism which should be further explored. Residents who grew up in the Northeast had decreases in their perspective-taking. Although regional differences in bias have been shown,35  less is known about regional differences in empathy.

Our study was not without limitations. First, measures used to evaluate explicit bias and empathy are subject to social desirability. However, comparisons of observer ratings on the IRI to self-report are strongly correlated.36  Second, the test-retest reliability of the IAT is moderate.37  Although we employed a pre-post design similar to prior educational research,38  and compared group IAT scores as opposed to individual scores,39  future research may consider using experimental designs or averaging multiple scores for each individual before determining pre-post differences across groups.37  Lastly, while there is limited information regarding the relationship between degree of change in bias or empathy and clinical outcomes, such as provider communication, patient satisfaction, and medical errors,17,40,41  it is unknown whether the changes demonstrated in our study would lead to behavior change. Direct observation through simulation or patient encounters will be important next steps. Qualitative methods may also provide deeper context42  and can be utilized in future evaluations.

Overall, our study demonstrates that resident participation in a longitudinal discussion-based curriculum addressing racism modestly reduced pediatric residents' racial preferences with minimal effects on empathy scales.

1. 
Maina
IW,
Belton
TD,
Ginzberg
S,
Singh
A,
Johnson
TJ.
A decade of studying implicit racial/ethnic bias in healthcare providers using the implicit association test
.
Soc Sci Med.
2018
;
199
:
219
-
229
.
2. 
Association of American Medical Colleges.
AAMC statement on police brutality and racism in America and their impact on health. Accessed September 14,
2021
.
3. 
Ludwig
S.
Domain of competence: professionalism
.
Acad Pediatri
.
2014
;
14
(suppl 2)
:
66
-
69
.
4. 
Brach
C,
Fraserirector
I.
Can cultural competency reduce racial and ethnic health disparities? A review and conceptual model
.
Med Care Res Rev
.
2000
;
57
(suppl 1)
:
181
-
217
.
5. 
Solchanyk
D,
Ekeh
O,
Saffran
L,
Burnett-Zeigler
IE,
Doobay-Persaud
A.
Integrating cultural humility into the medical education curriculum: strategies for educators
.
Teach Learn Med.
2021
;
33
(5)
:
554
-
560
.
6. 
Howell
BA,
Kristal
RB,
Whitmire
LR,
Gentry
M,
Rabin
TL,
Rosenbaum
J.
A systematic review of advocacy curricula in graduate medical education
.
J Gen Intern Med.
2019
;
34
(11)
:
2592
-
2601
.
7. 
Dupras
DM,
Wieland
ML,
Halvorsen
AJ,
Maldonado
M,
Willett
LL,
Harris
L.
Assessment of training in health disparities in US internal medicine residency programs
.
JAMA Netw Open
.
2020
;
3
(8)
:
e2012757
.
8. 
Tsai
JW,
Michelson
CD.
Implicit bias training in pediatric residency: attitudes amongst program directors and lessons learned from implementation
.
Acad Pediatr.
2017
;
17
(5)
:
e53
-
e54
.
9. 
Hassen
N,
Lofters
A,
Michael
S,
Mall
A,
Pinto
AD,
Rackal
J.
Implementing anti-racism interventions in healthcare settings: a scoping review
.
Int J Environ Res Public Health
.
2021
;
18
(6)
:
2993
.
10. 
Jones
CP.
Toward the science and practice of anti-racism: launching a national campaign against racism
.
Ethn Dis.
2018
;
28
(suppl 1)
:
231
-
234
.
11. 
Sternadori
M.
Empathy may curb bias: two studies of the effects of news stories on implicit attitudes toward African Americans and Native Americans
.
Contemporary Read Law Soc Justice
.
2017
;
9
(2)
:
11
-
27
.
12. 
Price
EG,
Beach
MC,
Gary
TL,
et al
A systematic review of the methodological rigor of studies evaluating cultural competence training of health professionals
.
Acad Med.
2005
;
80
(6)
:
578
-
586
.
13. 
Kern
DE,
Bass
EB,
Thomas
PA,
Howard
DM.
Curriculum Development for Medical Education: A Six Step Approach
.
Baltimore, MD
:
Johns Hopkins University Press;
1998
.
14. 
The Kirwan Institute.
2017 State of the Science: Implicit Bias Review
.
15. 
Newheiser
A,
Olson
KR.
White and Black American children's implicit intergroup bias
.
J Exp Soc Psychol
.
2012
;
48
(1)
:
264
-
270
.
16. 
Nosek
BA,
Smyth
FL.
A multitrait-multimethod validation of the implicit association test
.
Exp Psychol
.
2007
;
54
(1)
:
14
-
29
.
17. 
West
CP,
Huschka
MM,
Novotny
PJ,
et al
Association of perceived medical errors with resident distress and empathy: a prospective longitudinal study
.
JAMA.
2006
;
296
(9)
:
1071
-
1078
.
18. 
Davis
M.
A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
1980
;
10
(85)
:
1
-
19
.
19. 
Shankar
PR,
Piryani
RM.
Changes in empathy among first year medical students before and after a medical humanities module. Educ Med J.
2013
;
5
(1)
:
e35
-
e42
.
20. 
Drwecki
BB,
Moore
CF,
Ward
SE,
Prkachin
KM.
Reducing racial disparities in pain treatment: the role of empathy and perspective-taking
.
Pain
.
2011
;
152
(5)
:
1001
-
1006
.
21. 
U.S. Census Bureau.
Census regions and divisions of the United States. Accessed January 9,
2022
.
22. 
West
CP,
Dyrbye
L,
Satele
DV,
Sloan
JA,
Shanafelt
TD.
Concurrent validity of single-item measures of emotional exhaustion and depersonalization in burnout assessment
.
J Gen Intern Med.
2012
;
27
(11)
:
1445
-
1452
.
23. 
Devine
PG,
Forscher
PS,
Austin
AJ,
Cox
WT.
Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: a prejudice habit-breaking intervention
.
J Exp Soc Psychol
.
2012
;
48
(6)
:
1267
-
1278
.
24. 
Rudman
LA,
Ashmore
RD,
Gary
ML.
“Unlearning” automatic biases: the malleability of implicit prejudice and stereotypes
.
J Pers Soc Psychol.
2001
;
81
(5)
:
856
-
868
.
25. 
MedEdPORTAL. Anti-racism in Medicine Collection. Accessed January 9,
2022
.
26. 
Sawyer
J,
Gampa
A.
Implicit and explicit racial attitudes changed during Black Lives Matter
.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull.
2018
;
44
(7)
:
1039
-
1059
.
27. 
Gonzalez
CM,
Kim
MY,
Marantz
PR.
Implicit bias and its relation to health disparities: a teaching program and survey of medical students
.
Teach Learn Med.
2014
;
26
(1)
:
64
-
71
.
28. 
Burgess
D,
van Ryn
M,
Dovidio
J,
Saha
S.
Reducing racial bias among health care providers: lessons from social-cognitive psychology
.
J Gen Intern Med.
2007
;
22
(6)
:
882
-
887
.
29. 
Vorauer
JD,
Sasaki
SJ.
Helpful only in the abstract? Ironic effects of empathy in intergroup interaction
.
Psychol Sci.
2009
;
20
(2)
:
191
-
197
.
30. 
Brazeau
CM,
Schroeder
R,
Rovi
S,
Boyd
L.
Relationships between medical student burnout, empathy, and professionalism climate
.
Acad Med.
2010
;
85
(suppl 10)
:
33
-
36
.
31. 
Gonsalkorale
K,
Sherman
JW,
Klauer
KC.
Aging and prejudice: diminished regulation of automatic race bias among older adults
.
J Exp Soc Psychol
.
2009
;
45
(2)
:
410
-
414
.
32. 
Helson
R,
Jones
C,
Kwan
VS.
Personality change over 40 years of adulthood: hierarchical linear modeling analyses of two longitudinal samples
.
J Pers Soc Psychol
.
2002
;
83
(3)
:
752
-
766
.
33. 
Connor
P,
Sarafidis
V,
Zyphur
MJ,
Keltner
D,
Chen
S.
Income inequality and White-on-Black racial bias in the United States: evidence from project implicit and Google trends
.
Psychol Sci.
2019
;
30
(2)
:
205
-
222
.
34. 
Sabin
DJA,
Nosek
DBA,
Greenwald
DAG,
Rivara
DFP.
Physicians' implicit and explicit attitudes about race by MD race, ethnicity, and gender
.
J Health Care Poor Underserved
.
2009
;
20
(3)
:
896
-
913
.
35. 
Freng
S,
Schweitzer
K,
Estrada-Reynolds
V,
Leki
E,
Choi
S.
Geographic distribution of prejudice toward African Americans: applying the two-dimensional model
.
J Soc Psychol
.
2022
;
162
(2)
:
262
-
279
.
36. 
Saroglou
V,
Pichon
I,
Trompette
L,
Verschueren
M,
Dernelle
R.
Prosocial behavior and religion: new evidence based on projective measures and peer ratings
.
J Sci Study Rel
.
2005
;
44
(3)
:
323
-
348
.
37. 
Greenwald
AG,
Brendl
M,
Cai
H,
et al
The implicit association test at age 20: what is known and what is not known about implicit bias. University of Washington. Accessed May 5,
2022
.
38. 
Leslie
KF,
Sawning
S,
Shaw
MA,
et al
Changes in medical student implicit attitudes following a health equity curricular intervention
.
Med Teach
.
2018
;
40
(4)
:
372
-
378
.
39. 
Van Ryn
M,
Saha
S.
Exploring unconscious bias in disparities research and medical education
.
JAMA.
2011
;
306
(9)
:
995
-
996
.
40. 
Mitchell
G.
An implicit bias primer
.
Virginia J Soc Policy Law
.
2018
;
25
(1)
:
28
-
55
.
41. 
Penner
LA,
Dovidio
JF,
West
TV,
et al
Aversive racism and medical interactions with Black patients: a field study
.
J Exp Soc Psychol
.
2010
;
46
(2)
:
436
-
440
.
42. 
Jindal
M,
Mistry
KB,
McRae
A,
Unaka
N,
Johnson
T,
Thornton
RL.
“It makes me a better person and doctor”: a qualitative study of residents' perceptions of a curriculum addressing racism
.
Acad Pediatr.
2022
;
22
(2)
:
332
-
341
.

Author notes

Editor's Note: The online version of this article contains additional data and the surveys used in the study.

Funding: All phases of this study were supported by HRSA T0BHP28574 and NIH T32 HD094687, as well the Academic Pediatric Association's Young Investigator Award.

Competing Interests

Conflict of interest: The authors declare they have no competing interests.

Supplementary data